Memory
is very complex and hard to study because we can’t touch it, or physically see
it.
In its simplest mode, memory has two
components: Short Term Memory (STM) and
Long Term Memory (LTM).
STM
participates directly in cognitive processing (e.g. thinking, reflecting,
acting, and so on), but its size is very small.
LTM is used for permanent “storage” of information and experience.
One
popular view has it that the way people preserve relevance is by uncluttering
their minds, forgetting most of what they have learned. But this is not true in human cognition. Memory
does not deliberately flush out old information to make room for new, nor does
it overwhelm you with too much information when you are trying to recall
something. There is no problem (in everyday experience) with “remembering too
much” or “knowing too much.” We often
think highly of people with good memories, and pity those who are forgetful.
Forgetting is an aspect of memory, but the mechanism is not a purging but a
gradual fading over time through disuse.
Memory
has an extraordinary capacity for meaning, and thus for relevance. Unlike
computers, biological memory appears to be holographic in nature: a given piece of knowledge is not stored in a
single address or data field; it is distributed all over the brain and unlike
computers, human memory is associative in nature: a given piece of knowledge is accessed
through a rich network of semantic associations. Biological memory gains an
extraordinary degree of resiliency and flexibility through its holographic and
associative neural network mechanisms.
Studies
suggest that exercise increases memory (as there was an overall improvement of
test subjects after exercise). There are many factors involved that may link
exercise and memorization such as: increased blood flow which would bring more
oxygen to the brain, epinephrine production which would increase alertness, and
various hormone productions during exercise.
No comments:
Post a Comment